virus: Scanners....

Wade T.Smith (wade_smith@harvard.edu)
Thu, 24 Sep 98 13:16:24 -0400


So, how close does this make us? When do memes do their work, before
memories are stored, while they are stored, or after they are stored?.....

_______________________________________

Brain Scans Show How Memories are Formed

By William J. Cromie

Gazette Staff

The split-second birth of a memory has been captured with the aid of new
brain-visualization techniques. The resulting images can be used to
predict whether a specific experience will be remembered or forgotten.

"This has never been done before," said Anthony Wagner, a psychologist
at Harvard University. "We actually recorded what the brain was doing
during the fraction of a second that a memory is created. The levels of
brain activity we saw predicted whether or not an experience would
become part of long-term memory."

Wagner worked with other researchers from Harvard, Massachusetts General
Hospital in Boston, and Washington University in St. Louis to record
brain activity of people reading single words. The readers had to decide
whether a word represented a concrete object or an abstract concept,
i.e., "chair" or "charisma." They were not told to memorize the words,
or that their memories of the words would be tested.

Twenty minutes later, however, they were shown groups of words and asked
which ones they saw during the brain scanning. Their answers indicated
which words they remembered and which they forgot. Researchers then
compared levels of activity in various parts of their brains during
remembering and forgetting.

"This is the first work to tie the creation of a simple verbal memory to
specific levels of activity in certain areas of the brain," notes Daniel
Schacter, head of Harvard's Department of Psychology and a participant
in the research.

Most of the activity occurred in the left frontal lobe, located behind
the temple, and in the left temporal lobe, further back in the brain.
Specifically, much of the action took place in the inner wall of the
left temporal lobe in a structure called the parahippocamal cortex. That
structure lies on the pathway to the hippocampus, a part of the brain
that is vital for storing and retrieving memories.

"Even when two experiences are similarly novel, differences in
parahippocampal activity can predict whether one or both will be
remembered or forgotten," Wagner notes.

It takes less than a second for a simple memory like this to form, he
says.

Memorable Scenes

In complementary experiments at Stanford University in California,
researchers found that activity in the right frontal lobe and both left
and right temporal lobes predicts which seen scenes will be remembered.

"The two studies give us a more complete picture of how memories form,"
Wagner comments. "In the simplest terms, you can say verbal parts of an
experience appear to be encoded in left frontal regions; visual and
spatial parts of an experience appear to be encoded in right frontal
regions."

Word memory is intimately related to language ability and the latter
function rests in the left side of the brain for a majority of people.
In some left-handers, however, language sits on the right side; yet
others split language functions between the two sides. All of the people
in the Harvard and Stanford experiments were right-handed.

Details of both studies were reported in a recent issue of Science
magazine.

The big unanswered question is what causes the difference in brain
activity in the first place. Why did people in the Harvard study
remember some words and forget others? Why did individuals in the
Stanford study remember some scenic photos and not others?

The short answer, says Wagner, is "We don't know."

But being scientists, they have a theory. "It may be an interaction
between a person's prior experience and the time it takes to analyze a
word," Wagner speculates.

Some words, in other words, require retrieving more information from
memory than others. This is related to the time it takes a person to
decide whether a word represents something concrete or abstract. "The
more you attend to a word and think about its meaning, the greater the
level of brain activity and the more likely the word will be
remembered," Wagner says.

James Brewer of Stanford adds: "Perhaps one person sees a photograph of
Zion National Park and thinks, 'Hey I just visited that place on my way
to California.' Another person might think, 'Outdoor desert scene. . .
What's the next picture going to be?' The first individual's memory of
the picture is likely to be stronger."

Medical Applications

Wagner believes this kind of memory imaging could be used to predict the
onset of Alzheimer's disease. One of the first areas affected by the
malady is the middle of the temporal lobes, and that results in memory
loss. Changes in activity in this region might warn of coming problems
and enable treatment to begin at the earliest stages of the disease.

At present, no sure way exists to distinguish Alzheimer's from other
types of senile dementia. Autopsy provides the only definitive
diagnosis.

"Our experiment provides the most direct evidence to date about the role
of temporal regions in memory formation," Wagner says, "so it might help
spot signs of early brain changes that signal the onset of Alzheimer's."

The new memory-catching method may also provide a way to study natural
repair in a brain damaged by stroke. When a stroke strikes the left
frontal area, victims may lose all or part of their speech. Many,
however, regain at least some speaking ability when the right side of
the brain takes over this function. (There is evidence that women regain
lost language faster because they are less dependent on the left side
than men for language.)

"We believe that the right frontal area is wired-up in such a way that
it can take over functions on the left," Wagner explains. "If so, scans
of frontal region activity taken just after a stoke, and progressively
thereafter, should enable us to watch any functional reorganization that
occurs."

These methods might also be applied to learning about learning. Wagner
has begun a follow-up study to explore how the brain is involved in
learning and, perhaps, how to enhance the process.

"We know that attending to the meaning of something rather than its
appearance increases the probability that it will be remembered," Wagner
points out. He and his colleagues scanned the brains of people who were
asked to determine either the meaning of a word or whether it appeared
in lower or upper case. Concentrating on the meaning, they found,
produces more cerebral activity and results in better memory.

Other research shows that when people practice repeatedly to learn a new
skill, they do better if they take breaks often. "We want to find out
why spacing is so fruitful," says Wagner, "and to investigate how to
best structure learning and training sessions."

**************************************
Wade T. Smith
morbius@channel1.com
wade_smith@harvard.edu
**************************************